Nature, nurture and noise


The question of what makes each of us the persons we are has occupied philosophers, writers and daydreamers for millennia but has been open to scientific inquiry over a far shorter time. The answer clearly lies in the brain and, somehow, in how it is “wired” (whether that refers to the amount or type of connections between different brain areas or to differences in how circuits function). But differences in brain wiring could be either innate or due to experience or environmental effects. This has been famously framed, by Galton originally, as a clash of nature versus nurture. The inspiration for this phrase may have come from Shakepseare’s The Tempest, in which Prospero refers to Caliban as a "A devil, a born devil, on whose nature / Nurture can never stick". That line encapsulates the notion of an innate character that is resistant to extrinsic influences, especially efforts to change aspects of a person’s personality – an idea which anyone who has children may find easy to relate to.

While neurology and neuroscience have offered direct evidence that differences in the brain affect behaviour, it is behavioural genetics that is typically seen as having contributed most directly to the nature-nurture debate. Twin and adoption studies, the mainstays of behavioural genetics, have demonstrated very conclusively that many aspects of personality, behaviour or other psychological traits are highly heritable – that is, a large proportion of the variance in the trait across the population is attributable to differences in genes.

The logic of the twin studies is usually the inverse of the statement above – to look at people who share various proportions of their genes and see how similar they are to each other. These generally show that monozygotic (“identical”) twins are far more similar to each other for most psychological traits than dizogytic (“fraternal”) twins. Also, adoptive children tend to resemble their biological relatives for psychological traits and are hardly more similar to their adoptive family members than they would be to any stranger in the street.

These data have demonstrated unequivocally that variation in genes can affect behaviour in humans. They have also, however, dramatically illustrated the limits of such genetic effects. Monozygotic twins, while much more similar to each other than would be expected for people who don’t share all their genes in common, are nonetheless clearly not identical for most psychological traits. In fact, on average, genetic variance only explains about 50% of the phenotypic variance. What is causing the rest of the variance?

The presumption in the literature has been that it is something in the experience of the individuals – that in fact, the results of these behavioural genetic studies provide the strongest evidence to date for some effect of the environment. I say strongest evidence because in fact there is very little other evidence for direct experience-dependent effects on human psychological traits across the general population. (This is not in any way to deny the impact of factors such as serious abuse in individuals, which can be profound – it is just that, happily, such abuse is rare enough that it does not contribute significantly to the variance across the population).

Certainly, the twin and adoption studies have consistently found only a very modest effect of a shared family environment on the types of traits examined. The argument for interpreting the excess variance as being caused by experience is thus that it is the individual, “non-shared”, experiences that people have that make them different from each other. This has never made any sense to me, I have to confess. Non-shared experiences can make me differ from my twin but shared ones cannot make us more similar? An experience can affect my psychological development, but only if it does not also happen to my twin? Interactions with peers and teachers can have lasting effects on me but interactions with my parents cannot? Not only does this notion not make intuitive sense, there is no evidence for it. It seems far more likely that most non-traumatic experiences, regardless of who they are with, have little long-lasting effect on the kinds of traits that define us as persons. What then can account for the unexplained variance?

The basic problem with the interpretation above is that it limits “innate” influences to “genetic” ones. Just because some trait is not genetic does not mean it is not innate. If we are talking about how the brain gets wired, any number of prenatal environmental factors are known to have large effects. More interestingly, however, and probably a greater source of variance across the population, is intrinsic developmental variation. Wiring the brain is a highly complex procedure, reliant on cellular processes that are, in engineering terms, inherently “noisy”. Running the programme from the same starting point (a specific genotype) does not generate exactly the same output (the phenotype) every time. The effects of this noise are readily apparent at the anatomical level, when examining the impact of specific mutations, for example. In many cases, the phenotypic consequences are quite variable between genetically identical organisms, or even on two sides of the same brain. (If you want to see direct evidence of such developmental variation, take a directly face-on photograph of yourself, cut it in half and make mirror-image copies of the left and right sides. You will be amazed how different the two resultant faces are).

If the way the brain is wired is determined, not just by the starting genotype, but, to a large extent by chance events during development, then it is reasonable to expect this variation to be manifest in many psychological traits. Such traits may thus be far more innate than behavioural genetics studies alone would suggest. [Note that this does not imply all aspects of a person’s behaviour are innate and resistant to effects of experience – that is obviously a nonsense. It is important to recognize that the kinds of personality traits that have been examined in the studies mentioned above (such as extraversion or neuroticism) are basal characteristics, reflecting, for example, how strongly the brain responds to positive or to negative stimuli. These tendencies, in combination with later experience, influence, but in no way determine, moment-to-moment behaviour].

Happily, in my view, this places intrinsic limits on genetic determinism and the ability to predict many important aspects about a person from their genotype. Not limits based simply on our current knowledge that could one day be overcome – limits due to the inherently variable nature of neural development. Ultimately, what defines us each as persons, is thus dependent on nature, nurture and noise.

For more on this subject, see: Mitchell, K.J. (2007) The genetics of brain wiring; from molecule to mind. PLoS Biology Apr 17;5(4):e113. (Open Access).
http://www.plosbiology.org/article/info:doi/10.1371/journal.pbio.0050113

Comments

  1. Thanks. Very helpful.

    I was doing some research recently on twins and I realized that there a quite a few pairs where nobody seems to be sure if they are identical or not -- for example, the 7-foot-tall Lopez twins in the NBA. This can help explain this phenomenon of twins who seem to similar to be fraternal but not quite similar enough to be identical.

    ReplyDelete
  2. This comment has been removed by the author.

    ReplyDelete
  3. This comment has been removed by the author.

    ReplyDelete
  4. Interesting post. Well when there is so much a new. Thanks.
    college board .

    ReplyDelete
  5. it's like a state of mind!!! wonderful, most great visualization of this hymn. I glad////

    custom essay help

    ReplyDelete
  6. It is pretty impressive how the brain works when they are thinking in a specialized way. You can see it when they are thinking and the brain is analyzed. Also there is happing funny things when someone that it took Generic Viagra.

    ReplyDelete
  7. This is a great topic, it is true that it keep us wake at night thinking all of these things.
    best pharmacy

    ReplyDelete
  8. How a simple person become a philosopher or writer is one of the most interesting changes in the life of a person, for instance my Dad was a simple guard in xl pharmacy but he change to become a philosopher in Belgium

    ReplyDelete
  9. This can help explain this phenomenon of twins who seem to similar to be fraternal but not quite similar enough to be identical.
    xenical without prescription

    ReplyDelete
  10. I think more and more complex molecular patterns emerge through an interplay of mutual activation and repression
    company essay

    ReplyDelete
  11. You are our Goddess! Love you! i really admire your work. I must say thanks. -- Columbus Map

    ReplyDelete
  12. It is definitely all linked to the brain so much here. So much has to be researched on this so much here. I see so much coming from it all here. click here

    ReplyDelete
  13. Excellent and decent post. I found this much informative, as to what I was exactly searching for. Thanks for such post and keep it up. Very nice post. I just stumbled upon your blog and wanted to say that I have really enjoyed browsing your blog posts.
    online drugstore
    order nexium online

    ReplyDelete
  14. The first to apologize is the bravest. The first to forgive is the strongest. The first to move forward is the happiest.
    saree blouse

    ReplyDelete
  15. Very fine discussion of something that is rarely mentioned w.r.t. heritability.

    >Non-shared experiences can make me differ from my twin but shared >ones cannot make us more similar? ... It seems far more likely that >most non-traumatic experiences, regardless of who they are with, have >little long-lasting effect on the kinds of traits that define us as persons.

    Had to read this twice before I completely got the point. I think you have two main points:
    - In heritability, what is usually called the "non shared environment" is actually what's left over, everything else. It's virtually certain there's some developmental randomness that has significant effects on us, which would show up in this term, but we don't know how much.
    - If you believe the small contribution of shared environment (which most intellectuals who haven't drunk the heritability kool-aid have a hard time with), then it is indeed odd to suppose that "everything else" is what we would normally consider "environment", for example "good teacher", "bad teacher".

    Caveat about "non-traumatic" is important. From "Socioeconomic status modifies heritability of IQ in young children" http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/14629696
    >For families at the lowest levels of SES, shared
    >environment accounted for almost all of the variation in IQ,
    >with genes accounting for practically none.

    w.r.t. nature/nurture it is a good point that prenatal random developmental variation is indeed innate, even through it isn't heritable. This is where you start to realize how semantically tricky innateness and "other environment" are. For example, random developmental variation isn't going to stop at birth. Major neural remodelling takes place as late as adolescence, and we really have no idea whether randomness has effects on connectivity even in the relatively static adult brain. And if randomness is caused by a cosmic ray instead of thermal noise, then is that "environment?" Realistically, when people start talking about environment, they are talking about things that are perceptible and potentially under our control.

    Ultimately, nature/nurture rests on ideas about causality, determinism and free will. Connecting to these perennial philosophical disputes isn't going to solve any problems, but it can give new ways to look at things. People talk about the evils of genetic determinism, but is environmental determinism all that much better? What about those fuzzy intuitions we have about free will, self and authenticity?

    @robamacl http://humancond.org

    ReplyDelete
    Replies
    1. "with genes accounting for practically none"

      Has this study been replicated?

      Delete
  16. I like to read your website. You must write more posts about this article.

    Shuvo,
    Clipping Path

    ReplyDelete

Post a Comment

Popular posts from this blog

Undetermined - a response to Robert Sapolsky. Part 1 - a tale of two neuroscientists

Undetermined - a response to Robert Sapolsky. Part 2 - assessing the scientific evidence

Grandma’s trauma – a critical appraisal of the evidence for transgenerational epigenetic inheritance in humans