Why have genetic linkage studies of schizophrenia failed?


“If there really were rare, highly penetrant mutations that cause schizophrenia, linkage would have found them”. This argument is often trotted out in discussions of the genetic architecture of schizophrenia, which centre on the question of whether it is caused by rare, single mutations in most cases or whether it is due to unfortunate combinations of thousands of common variants segregating in the population. (Those are the two extreme starting positions).

It is true that many genetic linkage studies have been performed to look for mutations that are segregating with schizophrenia across multiple affected members in families. It is also true that these have been unsuccessful in identifying specific genes, but what does this tell us? Does it really rule out or even argue against the idea that most cases are caused by a single, rare mutation? (In the sense that, if the person did not have that mutation, they would not be expected to have the disorder).

This depends very much on the details of how these studies were carried out, their underlying assumptions, their specific findings and the real genetic architecture of the disorder. The idea of genetic linkage studies is that if you have a disease segregating in a particular family, you can use neutral genetic markers across the genome to look at the inheritance of different segments of chromosomes through the pedigree and track which ones co-segregate with the disease. For example, maybe all the affected children inherited a particular segment of chromosome 7 from mom, which can be tracked back to her dad and which is also carried by two of her brothers, who are affected, but not her sister, who is unaffected.

The problem is this: for each transmission from parent to child, 50% of the parent’s DNA is passed on (one copy of each chromosome, which is a shuffled version of the parent’s two copies of that chromosome – usually one segment from grandma, one from granddad, though sometimes there is a little more shuffling). If we only have one such transmission to look at, then we can only narrow down the region carrying a presumptive mutation to 50% of the genome – not much help really. In order for linkage studies to have power, you need to get data from many such transmissions and you therefore need big pedigrees – really huge pedigrees, actually, with information across multiple generations and preferably extending to lots of second or third degree relatives.

Where linkage studies of other diseases have been successful, those are the kinds of pedigrees that have been analysed. But they are not easy to find. That is especially true for schizophrenia, for a simple and tragic reason – this is a devastating disorder that strikes at an early age and causes very substantial impairment. It is associated with much higher mortality and drastically reduced fecundity (about a third the number of offspring), on average. The result is that people with the disorder tend to have fewer children and, if a mutation causing it is segregating in a pedigree, one would expect the pedigree to be smaller overall.

So, finding really large pedigrees where schizophrenia is clearly segregating across multiple generations has not been easy – in fact, there are very few reported that would be large enough by themselves to allow a highly powered linkage study.  (Here are some exceptions: Lindholm et al., 2001; Teltsh et al., 2008; Myles-Worsley et al. 2011)

Another thing that is absolutely imperative for linkage studies to work is that you know you are looking at the right phenotype – you must be certain of the affected status of each member of the pedigree. The analyses can tolerate misassignment of a few people, and can incorporate models of incomplete penetrance – where not all carriers of the mutation necessarily develop the disease. But if too many individuals are misassigned, the noise outweighs the signal. This is a particular problem for neuropsychiatric disorders, which we are now realising have highly overlapping genetic etiology. This is seen at the epidemiological level, in terms of shared risk across clinical categories, but also in the effects of particular, identified mutations, none of which is specific for a specific disorder. All the known mutations predispose to disease across diagnostic boundaries, manifesting in some people as schizophrenia, in others as bipolar disorder, autism, epilepsy, intellectual disability or other conditions.

Thus, schizophrenia does not typically “breed true” – there are few very large pedigrees where schizophrenia appears across multiple individuals in the absence of some other neuropsychiatric conditions in the family. Such mixed diagnosis pedigrees were typically excluded from linkage studies on the assumption that schizophrenia represents a natural kind at a genetic level. In fact, they might have been the most useful (and still could be) if what is tracked is neuropsychiatric disease more broadly. 

Given the scarcity of large pedigrees where schizophrenia was clearly segregating across multiple generations, researchers tried another approach, which is to find many smaller pedigrees and analyse them together. If schizophrenia is caused by the same mutation in different families across a population, this method should find it. That assumption holds for some simple Mendelian diseases where there is only or predominantly one genetic locus involved – such as Huntington’s disease or cystic fibrosis. But you can see what would happen to your study if it does not hold – if the disorder can in fact be caused by mutations in any of a large number of different genes – any real signals from specific families would be diluted by noise from all the other families.

Many such studies have been published, some combining very large numbers of families (in the hundreds). These have failed to localise any clearly consistent linkage regions, never mind specific genes, that harbour schizophrenia-causing mutations. This leads to one (and only one) very firm conclusion: schizophrenia is not like cystic fibrosis or Huntington’s disease – it is not caused by mutations at a single genetic locus or, indeed, at a small number of loci.

Nothing else can be concluded from these negative results. 

In particular, they do not argue against the possibility that schizophrenia is indeed caused by specific, single mutations in each individual or each family where it is segregating, if such mutations can occur at any of a large number of loci; i.e., if the disorder is characterised by very high genetic heterogeneity. This is not an outlandish model – one only has to look at conditions like intellectual disability, epilepsy, congenital deafness or various kinds of inherited blindness for examples of conditions that can be caused by mutations in dozens or even hundreds of different genes.

As it happens, the schizophrenia linkage studies have not necessarily been completely negative – many have found some positive linkages peaks, pointing to particular regions of the genome. These studies have not had the power to refine these signals down to a specific mutation, however, and most specific findings have not been replicated across other studies. It is therefore hard to tell if the statistical signals represent true or false positives in each study. But this lack of replication is to be expected under a model of extreme heterogeneity.

So, we can lay that argument to rest – the absence of evidence from linkage studies is not the evidence of anything – it does not, at least, bear on the current debate.

It should be stressed, however, that the failure of linkage also does not provide positive support for the model of extreme genetic heterogeneity – it is simply consistent with it. There are additional lines of evidence that argue against the most extreme version of the multiple rare variants model – the one that says each case is caused by a single mutation. I and others have argued that that is the best theoretical starting point and that we should complicate the model as necessary – but not more than necessary – to accommodate empirical findings (as opposed to jumping immediately to a massively polygenic model of inheritance, which has some very shaky underlying assumptions).

Such empirical findings include the incomplete penetrance of schizophrenia-associated mutations (which manifest as schizophrenia in only a percentage of carriers) and the range of additional phenotypes that they can cause. These findings suggest a prominent role for genetic modifiers – additional genetic variants in the background of each individual that modify the phenotypic expression of the primary mutation. This is to be expected – in fact, it is observed for even the most classically “Mendelian” disorders. In some cases, it may be impossible to even identify one mutation as “primary” – perhaps two or three mutations are required to really cause the disorder. Alternatively, some families, especially those with a very high incidence of mental illness, may have more than one causal mutation segregating, possibly coming from both parental lines (complicating linkage studies in just those families that look most promising).

One hope for finding causal mutations is the current technical ease and cost-effectiveness of sequencing the entire genome (or the part coding for proteins – the exome) of large numbers of individuals. The first whole-exome-sequencing study of schizophrenia has recently been published, with results that seem disappointing at first glance. The authors sequenced the exomes of 166 people with schizophrenia, identifying around a couple hundred very rare, protein-changing mutations in each person. This is normal – each of us typically carries that kind of burden of rare, possibly deleterious mutations. Finding which ones might be causing disease is the tricky bit – the hope is that multiple hits in the same gene(s) might emerge across the affected people. (This has been the case recently for similar studies of autism, with larger sample sizes and looking specifically for de novo, rather than inherited, mutations). No clear hits emerged from this study and follow-up of specific candidate mutations in a much larger sample did not provide strong support for any of them. (It should be stressed, this was a test for very particular mutations, not for the possible effects of any mutations in a given gene).

Again, we should be cautious about over-extrapolating from these negative data. The justified conclusion is that there are no moderately rare mutations segregating in the population that cause this disorder. These findings do not rule out (or even speak to) the possibility that the disease is caused by very rare mutations, specific instances of which would not be replicated in a wider population sample.

Much larger sequencing studies will be required to resolve this question. If, like intellectual disability, there are hundreds of genetic loci where mutations can result in schizophrenia, then samples of thousands of individuals will be required to find enough multiple hits to get good statistical evidence for any specific gene (allowing for heterogeneity of mutations at each locus). Such studies will emerge over the next couple of years and we will then be in a position to see how much more complicated our model needs to be. If even these larger studies fail to collar specific culprits, then we will have to figure out ways to resolve more complex genetic interactions that can explain the heritability of the disorder. For now, there are no grounds to reject the working model of extreme genetic heterogeneity with a primary, causal mutation in most cases.

Comments

  1. maybe being human is what genetically puts you at risk of schizophrenia

    :-)

    or maybe you are looking for something where you should be looking for nothing

    perhaps it is epigenetic

    and we are all at risk given the right (ie wrong) environmental conditions

    but please do continue

    p

    ReplyDelete
  2. Thanks for your comment. Regarding your first point, see also this recent post: http://wiringthebrain.blogspot.ie/2012/08/are-human-brains-especially-fragile.html

    As for why we are looking for something genetic, the reason is that family and twin studies show that schizophrenia is very highly heritable - that is, some people are more prone to it than others and the reason is largely genetic. So, there's something to explain - it's finding the specific genetic differences involved that has been the tricky bit!

    ReplyDelete
  3. Thanks for a very informative explanation.
    I think that some lessons should be taken from hearing loss.
    The complex heritabilty of hearing loss might be a good model to the neurodevelopmental\psychiatric disorders.

    But a more important lesson is the phenotype. In the past deaf people were considered deaf&dumb. In fact, deaf people became mentally retarded as a result of their social isolation. Preventing isolation by simply teaching deaf people to communicate prevented their mental retardation. A good reading about it is Seeing Voices by Oliver Sacks.
    the lesson is that to understand the genetics of hearing loss the hearing should be studied not the mental retardation.
    What I am trying to say is that although some psychiatric disorders are heritable, they are also acquired.
    In order to understand their etiology we need to distinguish between the innate and the acquired phenotypes.

    ReplyDelete
    Replies
    1. Thanks Guy for that perspective. I think you have hit the nail on the head. Right now we diagnose schizophrenia based on a person's behaviour and reports of their subjective experience, which point to a recognisable cluster of symptoms with a characteristic progression, which we label as schizophrenia. But this is clinically very heterogeneous and we know it is also etiologically very heterogeneous - it could be arrived at through many different routes. Currently, we know very little of what those routes might be, but accepting the heterogeneity is key to making progress.

      Delete
  4. I would say that 3 decades of failed genetic linkage studies should at least raise the question of whether genes have anything to do with schizophrenia. Each time a new genetic linkage study comes out, it is often front page news that "gene for schizophrenia" has been found. We already know that no such gene exists and it is getting harder and harder to make the case ever for a miniscule association between a genetic loci and schizophrenia. We have had 30 years of false positives. These studies are poorly designed, generally without a control, and are replete with false positives that are never replicated. It's not just schizophrenia. The same can be said for any mental disorder. Not one gene has been definitively linked to any mental disorder to date. It may be paradoxical, but it is time to consider the possibility that these disorders are not related to genes. True, heritability has generally been demonstrated, but heritability does not prove that it is genetic. Feel free to check my new blog out for some discussion of specific genetic linkage studies. It is a work in progress, but I have a few posts: www.genesareoverrated.blogspot.com

    ReplyDelete
  5. Thanks very much for making this information available. I love reading stuff like this as it helps me understand the causes of my condition better, although I have no scientific knowledge so a lot of the information out there is difficult for me to understand. Thank you for making this so accessable.
    I've always felt, from my own experience, that genes are the main cause of schizophrenia. I believe this because I have suffered for my entire life, and there are so many instances of mental illness in my family. I believe that one day science will find the cause of the problem, and we will be on the way to finding an effective treatment. Keep up the good work! Emily

    ReplyDelete
    Replies
    1. Thanks, I really appreciate the positive feedback and also your sharing your personal experience.

      Delete
  6. Very informative stuff! thanks for sharing the blog.I wish you to post the new updates regularly.

    best seo services

    ReplyDelete
  7. There is definitely so many people dealing with this so much here. So much more research has to be done done with it. I hope we can get a resolution to this here.
    click here

    ReplyDelete
  8. Hope there would more twin studies for schizophrenia and we would manage at last to find the specific genetic differences involved that has been the tricky bit! Thanks a lot Kevin for the efforts.Kevin Right

    ReplyDelete

Post a Comment

Popular posts from this blog

Undetermined - a response to Robert Sapolsky. Part 1 - a tale of two neuroscientists

Undetermined - a response to Robert Sapolsky. Part 2 - assessing the scientific evidence

Grandma’s trauma – a critical appraisal of the evidence for transgenerational epigenetic inheritance in humans