Bad to the bone; altered connections in the brains of psychopaths

The manipulative con-man.  The guy who lies to your face, even when he doesn’t have to.  The child who tortures animals.  The cold-blooded killer.  

psychopath

Psychopaths are characterised by an absence of empathy and poor impulse control, with a total lack of conscience.  About 1% of the total population can be defined as psychopaths, according to a detailed psychological profile checklist.  They tend to be egocentric, callous, manipulative, deceptive, superficial, irresponsible and parasitic, even predatory.  The majority of psychopaths are not violent and many do very well in jobs where their personality traits are advantageous and their social tendencies tolerated.  However, some have a predisposition to calculated, “instrumental” violence; violence that is cold-blooded, planned and goal-directed.  Psychopaths are vastly over-represented among criminals; it is estimated they make up about 20% of the inmates of most prisons.  They commit over half of all violent crimes and are 3-4 times more likely to re-offend.  They are almost entirely refractory to rehabilitation.  These are not nice people. 

So how did they get that way?  Is it an innate biological condition, a result of social experience, or an interaction between these factors?  Longitudinal studies have shown that the personality traits associated with psychopathy are highly stable over time.  Early warning signs including “callous-unemotional traits” and antisocial behaviour can be identified in childhood and are highly predictive of future psychopathy.  Large-scale twin studies have shown that these traits are highly heritable – identical twins, who share 100% of their genes, are much more similar to each other in this trait than fraternal twins, who share only 50% of their genes.  In one study, over 80% of the variation in the callous-unemotional trait across the population was due to genetic differences.  In contrast, the effect of a shared family environment was almost nil.  Psychopathy seems to be a lifelong trait, or combination of traits, which are heavily influenced by genes and hardly at all by social upbringing.

The two defining characteristics of psychopaths, blunted emotional response to negative stimuli, coupled with poor impulse control, can both be measured in psychological and neuroimaging experiments.  Several studies have found decreased responsiveness of the amygdala to fearful or other negative stimuli in psychopaths.  They do not seem to process heavily loaded emotional words, like “rape”, for example, any differently from how they process neutral words, like “table”.  This lack of response to negative stimuli can be measured in other ways, such as the failure to induce a galvanic skin response (heightened skin conduction due to sweating) when faced with an impending electrical shock.  Psychopaths have also been found to underactivate limbic (emotional) regions of the brain during aversive learning, correlating with an insensitivity to negative reinforcement.  The psychopath really just doesn’t care.  In this, psychopaths differ from many people who are prone to sudden, impulsive violence, in that those people tend to have a hypersensitive negative emotional response to what would otherwise be relatively innocuous stimuli. 

What these two groups have in common is poor impulse control.  This faculty relies on the part of the brain called the prefrontal cortex, most particularly the orbitofrontal cortex.  It is known that lesions to this part of the brain impair planning, prediction of consequences, and inhibition of socially unacceptable behaviour – the cognitive mechanisms of “free won’t”, rather than free will.  This brain region is also normally activated by aversive learning, and this activation is also reduced in psychopaths.  In addition, both the prefrontal cortex and the amygdala show substantial average reductions in size in psychopaths, suggesting a structural difference in their brains. 

These findings have now been united by a recent study that directly analysed connectivity between these two regions.  Using diffusion tensor imaging (see post of August 31st 2009), Craig and colleagues found that a measure of the integrity of the axonal tract connecting these two regions, called the uncinate fasciculus, was significantly reduced in psychopaths.  Importantly, connectivity of these regions to other parts of the brain was normal.  These data thus suggest a specific disruption of the network connecting orbitofrontal cortex and amygdala in psychopaths, the degree of which correlated strongly with the subjects’ scores on the psychopathy checklist. 

All of these findings are pointing to a picture of psychopathy as an innate, genetically driven difference in connectivity between parts of the brain that normally drive empathy, conscience and impulse control.  Not a fault necessarily, and not something that could be classified as a disease or that is always a disadvantage.  At a certain frequency in the population, the traits of psychopathy may be highly advantageous to the individual.

This conclusion has serious ethical and legal implications.  Could a psychopath mount a legal defense by saying “my brain made me do it”?  Or my “genes made me do it”?  Is this any different from saying my rotten childhood made me do it?  Psychopaths know right from wrong – they just don’t care.  That is what society calls “bad”, not “mad”.  But if they are constitutionally incapable of caring, can they really be blamed for it?  On the other hand, if violent psychopaths are a continuing danger to society and completely refractory to rehabilitation, what is to be done with them?  Perhaps, as has been proposed in the UK, people with the extreme psychopathic personality profile (or maybe in the near future even a specific genetic profile?) should be monitored or segregated even before they commit a crime. 

While it is crucial that these debates are informed by good science, these issues have no clear-cut answers.  They will be resolved on a pragmatic basis, weighing the behaviour that society is willing to tolerate versus the rights of the individual, whatever their brains look like, to define their own moral standards. 

 

 

Craig, M., Catani, M., Deeley, Q., Latham, R., Daly, E., Kanaan, R., Picchioni, M., McGuire, P., Fahy, T., & Murphy, D. (2009). Altered connections on the road to psychopathy Molecular Psychiatry, 14 (10), 946-953 DOI:

Comments

  1. Surely the job of the legal system is too prevent people being a harm to society. "My genes made me do it" is not a *defense* it's just an explanation. If someone is acting or behaving in a way that is dangerous, they need to be placed somewhere where they will not be a danger; whether that's in prison, or somewhere they too can be helped, if such help is needed.

    "My brain made me do it" suggests quite a sophisticated level of mind/brain duality that I'm not quite sure would pass in the legal system, given that philosophers are still trying to work it out :)

    ReplyDelete
  2. Does this mean that the Ludovico treatment actually would not be very effective?

    ReplyDelete
  3. Actually, "my brain made me do it" is essentially a not guilty by reason of insanity plea. Wherever you stand philosophically, the legal system accepts this as a viable exculpatory factor. Not in the sense of saying that the accused didn't so something; just that they can't be held "guilty" of it, in the legal sense, due to diminished capacity. This does not currently extend to psychopaths who can be said to know the difference between right and wrong, at least on an intellectual level. However, psychopaths may be so impaired in moral reasoning as to not FEEL the difference between right and wrong. They are driven by self-interest and, it could be claimed, are "morally insane". As I said, this does not mean that violent psychopaths should be set free - the facts that they are highly likely to reoffend and unlikely to be reformed leads to the utilitarian conclusion that society should be protected from them.

    ReplyDelete
  4. Psychopathic doctors always end up at the top of the academic ladder. They use patients for their own advancement and also their own emotional needs. More oversight of physicians is needed as well as better initial screening.

    A healthcare system that values patient experiences rather than the physicians' psychopathology will need to be implemented if psychopaths are to be prevented from causing harm.

    ReplyDelete
  5. The legal system itself is psychopathic, completely lacking empathy or compassion. The irony is that in order to protect society from psychopathy it must be allowed to make decisions based on emotion. Psychopathy can only be identified by patterns of behavior. Psychopaths easily fool Judges and psychological experts by getting them to focus on the individual behavior. They will lie about thier use of instrumental agression and they will blame the victim in trying to justify their extreme response.

    It is the pattern that identifies the psychopath. In order to identify the behavior as a pattern, one must make a moral and emotional judgement about the collective behaviors.

    The ONLY way to protect society from psychopaths is to screen for them upon entry into the 'system'. I would propose that certain crimes or legal events trigger required fMRI or PET testing, IF fMRI and PET imaging can identify psychopathy with accuracy.

    ReplyDelete
  6. IMO, they shouldn't be imprisoned, but just marked as different, in a visible manner.

    Some sort of small, scary,sharp looking tattoo on one cheek.

    They'd find it harder to cheat on people, because everyone would be wary of them, and concentrate when dealing with them.

    ReplyDelete
  7. Kevin, could you please share the source of the statistics you cite in the first paragraph? I'm interested to know if the dataset allows for international generalizations. Thanks!

    ReplyDelete
  8. Those particular statistics are taken from a review by Glenn and Raine (2008) in the Psychiatric Clinics of North America. "The Neurobiology of Psychopathy". (They refer to work by Bob Hare)

    See also: "Psychopathy as a Clinical and Empirical Construct" by Hare and Neumann (2008) in Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol.

    ReplyDelete
  9. Great post. I picked up a reference to this post in my blog. Thanks for the cool read.

    ReplyDelete
  10. Hi,

    I Saw Your Deatails on the internet i see your profile and would like you to come on my uk radio show thursdays for 1 hr 45 mins from 1 pm to 245 pm eastern time.

    My show is the freedom of speech show pls add me to skype as thefreedomofspeechshow i am Andy Peacher we do have a usa calling in telephone number the station i am on is www.criticalmassradio..co.uk

    we have dates from 30th may available but also i am starting a usa show very soon on www.oriontalkradio.com so more dates will become avilable very soon.

    When the interview is complete you get a free mp3 a u tube a mix cloud of the show.

    Andy

    ReplyDelete
  11. I think more updates and will be returning. I have filtered for qualified edifying substance of this calibre all through the past various hours. www.flevoservice.co.uk |

    www.ascotuw.co.uk |

    www.stagandhendays.co.uk |

    www.carvinworld.co.uk |

    www.ashun.co.uk |

    www.signvinylonline.co.uk |

    www.kickstartup.co.uk |

    www.radioalternativa.co.uk |

    www.mob-apps.co.uk |

    www.kellysofnarberth.co.uk |

    ReplyDelete
  12. Are you an inquisitive member of industry or scientist interested in learning more about fMRI? Attend the upcoming fMRI Course at the Mind Research Network in Alb, NM on May 29-31, 2014. Space is limited. Go to http://www.mrn.org/education-outreach/courses-and-events/ for more information.

    ReplyDelete

Post a Comment

Popular posts from this blog

Grandma’s trauma – a critical appraisal of the evidence for transgenerational epigenetic inheritance in humans

Undetermined - a response to Robert Sapolsky. Part 1 - a tale of two neuroscientists

Undetermined - a response to Robert Sapolsky. Part 2 - assessing the scientific evidence